The Relative Income Hypothesis: A comparison of methods.

Similar documents
Household Finances and Well-Being: An Empirical Analysis of Comparison Effects. Sarah Brown Daniel Gray ISSN

Household Finances and Well-Being: An Empirical Analysis of Comparison Effects

Comparison Income Effect on Subjective Well-Being

How exogenous is exogenous income? A longitudinal study of lottery winners in the UK

Inter-ethnic Marriage and Partner Satisfaction

Household Finances, Financial Satisfaction and Subjective. Prosperity: An Empirical Analysis of Comparison Effects

Does Growth make us Happier? A New Look at the Easterlin Paradox

Age, Life-satisfaction, and Relative Income

Relative Income and Hours Worked: Empirical Evidence from the US

Subjective Financial Situation and Overall Life Satisfaction: A Joint Modelling Approach

How Does Education Affect Mental Well-Being and Job Satisfaction?

Explaining the Easterlin paradox

Unemployment and Happiness

The Social Costs of Unemployment: Accounting for Unemployment Duration

Age, Life-Satisfaction, and Relative Income: Insights from the UK and Germany

JOB SATISFACTION AND FAMILY HAPPINESS: THE PART-TIME WORK PUZZLE*

Explaining procyclical male female wage gaps B

Long-run Effects of Lottery Wealth on Psychological Well-being. Online Appendix

Wage Scarring The problem of a bad start. by Robert Raeside, Valerie Edgell and Ron McQuaid

This is a repository copy of Part time employment and happiness: A cross-country analysis.

Does Income Inequality Impact Individual Happiness? Evidence from Canada

Income Comparisons and Subjective Well-Being: Evidence from Self- Perceived Relative Income Data from Chinese Elderly People

Understanding the subjective consequences of early job insecurity in Europe

This is a repository copy of Asymmetries in Bank of England Monetary Policy.

THE PERSISTENCE OF UNEMPLOYMENT AMONG AUSTRALIAN MALES

Labor Participation and Gender Inequality in Indonesia. Preliminary Draft DO NOT QUOTE

Income level of reference group and subjective welfare - Verification of the relative income hypothesis - *

An Empirical Examination of Traditional Equity Valuation Models: The case of the Athens Stock Exchange

Money illusion under test

Macroeconomic Preferences by Income and Education Level: Evidence from Subjective Well-Being Data

Julio Videras Department of Economics Hamilton College

Subjective well-being: Keeping up with the perception of the Joneses.

HYPERTENSION AND LIFE SATISFACTION: A COMMENT AND REPLICATION OF BLANCHFLOWER AND OSWALD (2007)

Adaptation, Anticipation and Social Interactions in Happiness: An Integrated Error-Correction Approach. Maarten Vendrik Maastricht University IZA

The persistence of urban poverty in Ethiopia: A tale of two measurements

Happiness and House Prices in Canada:

Do people adapt to changes in income and other circumstances? The discussion is not finished yet.

ONLINE APPENDIX (NOT FOR PUBLICATION) Appendix A: Appendix Figures and Tables

who needs care. Looking after grandchildren, however, has been associated in several studies with better health at follow up. Research has shown a str

Should a government target real income growth or devote more resource's to the reduction of unemployment? Which will yield more happiness?

Economic Development and Subjective Well-Being. An in-depth study based on VARHS 2012

Happiness across the life span:

The Impact of Self-Employment Experience on the Attitude towards Employment Risk

a. Explain why the coefficients change in the observed direction when switching from OLS to Tobit estimation.

Relative income and happiness in Asia: Evidence from nationwide surveys in China, Japan, and Korea

ECONOMETRICS OF PANEL DATA Michele Cincera

DOES RELATIVE INCOME MATTER? ARE THE CRITICS RIGHT?

THE DISTRIBUTION AND DYNAMICS OF ECONOMIC AND SOCIAL WELL-BEING IN THE UK:

The Index of Happiness and Economic Growth

Who compares to whom? The anatomy of income comparisons in Europe

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES NEW MEASURES OF THE COSTS OF UNEMPLOYMENT: EVIDENCE FROM THE SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING OF 2.3 MILLION AMERICANS

ABSTRACT. Asian Economic and Financial Review ISSN(e): ISSN(p): DOI: /journal.aefr Vol. 9, No.

The Impact of a $15 Minimum Wage on Hunger in America

Understanding the underlying dynamics of the reservation wage for South African youth. Essa Conference 2013

An Empirical Note on the Relationship between Unemployment and Risk- Aversion

HOUSEHOLDS INDEBTEDNESS: A MICROECONOMIC ANALYSIS BASED ON THE RESULTS OF THE HOUSEHOLDS FINANCIAL AND CONSUMPTION SURVEY*

This is a repository copy of Financial Expectations, Consumption and Saving: A Microeconomic Analysis.

Do Domestic Chinese Firms Benefit from Foreign Direct Investment?

Testing the Tunnel Effect: Comparison, Age and Happiness in UK and German Panels. School of Economics & Finance Discussion Papers

Online Appendix Long-Lasting Effects of Socialist Education

Fertility Decline and Work-Life Balance: Empirical Evidence and Policy Implications

Review questions for Multinomial Logit/Probit, Tobit, Heckit, Quantile Regressions

Working Paper No Time and Money: Substitutes in Real Terms and Complements in Satisfactions

Factors in the returns on stock : inspiration from Fama and French asset pricing model

Equality and Fertility: Evidence from China

Retirement and Subjective Well-Being

econstor Make Your Publications Visible.

The persistence of regional unemployment: evidence from China

Nominal or Real? The Impact of Regional Price Levels on Satisfaction with Life

Can Subjective Well-Being Predict Unemployment Length?

Financial liberalization and the relationship-specificity of exports *

Happy Voters. Exploring the Intersections between Economics and Psychology. Federica Liberini 1, Eugenio Proto 2 Michela Redoano 2.

Pension Wealth and Household Saving in Europe: Evidence from SHARELIFE

The effect of earnings on housework: Pros and cons of HILDA's time use data items

Household Use of Financial Services

State Dependence in a Multinominal-State Labor Force Participation of Married Women in Japan 1

Retirees perceptions of quality of life

Cross-country Differences in Well-being Consequences of Unemployment in Europe

Homeownership and subjective well-being

WELFARE REFORM AND THE BEHAVIOUR OF THE UNEMPLOYED. Sarah Brown and Karl Taylor Department of Economics University Of Sheffield InstEAD and IZA

While real incomes in the lower and middle portions of the U.S. income distribution have

DYNAMICS OF URBAN INFORMAL

Wage Gap Estimation with Proxies and Nonresponse

The current study builds on previous research to estimate the regional gap in

Unhappiness and Job Finding

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES MAKING SENSE OF THE LABOR MARKET HEIGHT PREMIUM: EVIDENCE FROM THE BRITISH HOUSEHOLD PANEL SURVEY

Fairness and Incentive Contracting Based on the Performance Budget: Testing Experiment on Referent Cognition Theory

Financial capability, income and psychological wellbeing

Social capital Predicts Happiness over Time: Evidence from Macro and Micro Data

The economic value of key intermediate qualifications: estimating the returns and lifetime productivity gains to GCSEs, A levels and apprenticeships

Can Subjective Well-Being Predict Unemployment Duration?

Strictness of Tax Compliance Norms: A Factorial Survey on the Acceptance of Inheritance Tax Evasion in Germany

Claudia Senik. (Paris-Jourdan Sciences Economiques and University Paris-IV Sorbonne)

UNIVERSITY OF WAIKATO. Hamilton New Zealand. An Illustration of the Average Exit Time Measure of Poverty. John Gibson and Susan Olivia

CFCM CFCM CENTRE FOR FINANCE AND CREDIT MARKETS. Working Paper 12/01. Financial Literacy and Consumer Credit Use. Richard Disney and John Gathergood

Moral hazard in a voluntary deposit insurance system: Revisited

Corporate Social Responsibility and Financing Constraints: Empirical Evidence from China s Listed Corporates. Xilun Zhu

Mandated Labour Protections & Government Safety Nets: Economic outcomes and worker security

Income Satisfaction Inequality and its Causes

A Canonical Correlation Analysis of Financial Risk-Taking by Australian Households

Transcription:

The Relative Income Hypothesis: A comparison of methods. Sarah Brown, Daniel Gray and Jennifer Roberts ISSN 1749-8368 SERPS no. 2015006 March 2015

The Relative Income Hypothesis: A comparison of methods. Sarah Brown, Daniel Gray and Jennifer Roberts * Department of Economics University of Sheffield 9 Mappin Street Sheffield S1 4DT United Kingdom Abstract: Empirical studies of the relative income hypothesis have found both positive and negative effects of relative income on utility. Differences in data and methods make the results difficult to compare. To facilitate comparisons we explore the problem using a large UK household panel. Our findings highlight the sensitivity of the estimated relative income effect to the definition of the reference group and to the estimation strategy employed. Given the increasing attention paid to interdependent preferences in the economics literature, and the implications for problems such as the measurement of societal welfare, our findings are of interest for both the theoretical and empirical study of the relative income hypothesis. JEL: I31 Key Words: relative income, reference group, subjective well-being. *Corresponding Author. Email j.r.roberts@sheffield.ac.uk Tel: +44 114 222 3403 Data Acknowledgment: Understanding Society is an initiative by the Economic and Social Research Council, with scientific leadership by the Institute for Social and Economic Research, University of Essex, and survey delivery by the National Centre for Social Research. 1

1. Introduction and Background The relative income hypothesis was proposed by Duesenberry (1949) to explain savings behaviour in the US. The hypothesis, which states that individual utility depends both on own income and on income relative to that of others, did not attract a lot of empirical attention until two separate later developments. Firstly, Kahneman and Tversky (1979) provided a theoretical justification for the importance of comparison effects by explaining that changes from a reference point mattered for decisions, not absolute states of wealth. One possible reference point was the income of a comparison group of others. Secondly, the rise of happiness economics began to persuade economists that self-reported measures of wellbeing could be used as reliable proxies for individual utility (see for example Clark and Oswald, 1994) 1. There are numerous studies of the relative income effect but, while own income is generally found to have a positive effect on utility, there is no consensus as to the sign on relative income. Theoretical arguments can be made for both a negative sign via comparison effects, and a positive sign via information effects (Senik, 2004). Given the importance of the relative income hypothesis, for example in understanding societal welfare or as a potential explanation for the Easterlin paradox (Easterlin, 1974), it is a serious shortcoming that the empirical literature raises more questions than answers. In an effort to highlight some of these issues we use data from a UK household longitudinal data set to test the relative income hypothesis in a number of ways. The basic model is: U it = α + βy it + γy it r + θ k x k,it + ε it i subscripts the individual and t, time. U is a proxy for utility, such as self-reported happiness or life satisfaction. y is own income, y r is relative income (the income of the reference group), x is a set of k conditioning variables and is the error term. Hence the main parameter of interest is. Partly the lack of consensus on the sign of arises because it is difficult to make comparisons across the empirical literature due to differences in data, definitions, model specification and estimation methods. Firstly, data for many different countries have been used, with different average income levels, as well as both cross section and longitudinal data. Secondly, estimation methods depend on the type of data and form of the utility proxy; specifically some studies control for individual unobserved heterogeneity and some do not, and studies vary in the way they deal with the ordinal nature of many of the dependent variables. Thirdly, different ways of defining the reference group; sometimes this is defined on the basis of individual characteristics ( people like you ) and sometimes it is defined spatially ( people k 1 It is worth noting here that while the subjective well-being literature is our focus in this paper, alternative approaches to studying reference group effects do exist. For example Card et al. (2012) utilise a field experiment on knowledge of colleagues pay, and Brown et al. (2008) carry out a laboratory experiment with students on future hypothetical wage distributions. 2

near you ). These methods can also be combined in various ways so that the comparison group is local people with similar characteristics, who might be work colleagues, old school friends, relatives etc 2. Finally, different proxies for individual utility are used. Table 1 summarises key papers from the empirical literature, describing the data, utility proxy, method for defining the reference group and estimation method. This table clearly illustrates the lack of agreement on the direction of the relative income effect. 2. Empirical Analysis We analyse the first three waves of Understanding Society, the UK household longitudinal study, covering 2009 to 2013 (University of Essex, 2012). We use data for all adults, yielding an unbalanced panel of 40,335 individuals (99,430 individual/wave observations). The average age is 48 years and 56% are female. We specify two utility proxies (U), which have been analysed in the existing literature, overall life satisfaction and the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ). Overall life satisfaction is based on the question, Please tick the number which you feel best describes how dissatisfied or satisfied you are with your life overall. This is measured on a 7 point scale, where 1 indicates completely dissatisfied and 7 completely satisfied ; the average score is 5.21. The GHQ measure of psychological wellbeing is constructed by summing the responses to 12 questions and is measured on a 36 point scale. In our data higher values represent better well-being, and the average score is 24.92. Income (y) is based on nominal gross household income in the month prior to the interview; we omit households who report zero income. We explore two measures of relative income (y r ) replicating methods that have been used in the literature. Firstly, using individual characteristics the reference group is based on age categories (<25, 25-34, 35-44, 45-65, 66>), education (no qualification, other qualification, GCSE or equivalent, A-level or equivalent, degree or higher) and gender. Secondly, the reference group is based on a spatial definition, specifically the average income in 405 local authority districts (LAD) in a particular year. 3 Table 2 summarises the results for the relative income effect for the two dependent variables and the two reference group definitions. The additional control variables (x k ) are listed in the note to Table 2. To explore the robustness of the results to estimation method we model ordinal life satisfaction in four ways: (1) pooled ordered probit; (2) random effects (RE) ordered probit; (3) RE ordered probit with Mundlak transformation to allow for unobserved time invariant effects; and (4) fixed effects (FE) ordered logit. Similarly, we model the continuous GHQ measure by pooled OLS, as well as model with RE and FE. 2 Another strand of the literature has also explored whether it is average income of the reference group, or the individual s position in the ranking of incomes that is the driving factor in determining well-being (see for example Card et al. 2012; and Brown et al. 2008). 3 Our findings are robust to other spatially defined reference groups including 240 Travel to Work Areas and 12 Government Office Regions. 3

It is apparent that own income effects are, as expected, positive regardless of the dependent variable and estimation technique. In contrast, both the sign and significance of the relative income effect varies with reference group and estimation technique. For both dependent variables, with the reference group based on individual characteristics, highly statistically significant negative relative income effects are apparent with the exception of the FE estimates, where the effects are insignificant. When the reference group is defined spatially the pattern of results is less pronounced. Although the estimated effects are all positive, statistical significance varies across the estimation methods. In the case of life satisfaction, only the RE ordered probit and FE ordered logit estimates attain statistical significance. In contrast, for the GHQ, the positive relative income effects are strongly significant in the case of pooled and RE estimates. By way of interpretation, own income and income of the reference group is much more closely correlated when the reference group is defined via individual characteristics (r = 0.419) than when it is defined spatially (r = 0.239). While Senik (2004) has argued that a positive coefficient is evidence of a dominant information effect ; it seems equally likely that this coefficient is picking up area wealth effects when a spatially defined reference group is used. 3. Conclusion To summarise, our findings highlight the sensitivity of the estimated relative income effect to both the definition of the reference group and to the estimation strategy employed. In particular, the way in which unobserved heterogeneity is allowed for, which in turn is constrained by the nature of the utility proxy used and the way the reference group is defined, is of crucial importance to measuring and interpreting the relative income effect. Given the increasing attention paid to the role of comparison income and interdependent preferences in the economics literature, and the implications for a number of key concepts such as the measurement of societal welfare, authors have an obligation to make a convincing theoretical case for their choices and/or to illustrate the empirical robustness of their results. Much more attention should also be paid to exploring how the reference group is constructed, and data such as the Understanding Society best friends module could facilitate this work in the future. Finally, both field and laboratory experiments on reference group effects can be explored as an alternative to the survey based subjective well-being approach. 4

References Brown, G.D., Gardner, J., Oswald, A.J., Qian, J., (2008). Does wage rank affect employees well-being?. Industrial Relations, 47, 355 389. Card, D., (2012). Inequality at work: The effect of peer salaries on job satisfaction. American Economic Review, 102, 2981 3003. Clark, A.E., Oswald, A. (1994). Unhappiness and unemployment. Economic Journal 104(424), 648-659. Clark, A. E., Oswald, A. J. (1996). Satisfaction and comparison income. Journal of Public Economics, 61(3), 359-381. Clark, A. E., Westergård Nielsen, N., Kristensen, N. (2009). Economic satisfaction and income rank in small neighbourhoods. Journal of the European Economic Association, 7(2 3), 519-527. Duesenberry, J.S. (1949) Income, Saving and the Theory of Consumer Behaviour. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Easterlin, R.,(1974) Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence in David, P.A. Reder, M.W. eds. Nations and households in economic growth. Academic Press. Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2005). Income and well-being: An empirical analysis of the comparison income effect. Journal of Public Economics, 89(5), 997-1019. Kahneman, D., Tversky, A., (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263 291. Luttmer, E. F. (2005). Neighbours as negatives: Relative earnings and well-being. The Quarterly journal of economics, 120(3), 963-1002. Mangyo, E., Park, A. (2011). Relative deprivation and health which reference groups matter?. Journal of Human Resources, 46(3), 459-481. McBride, M. (2001). Relative-income effects on subjective well-being in the cross-section. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 45(3), 251-278. Senik, C. (2004). When information dominates comparison: Learning from Russian subjective panel data. Journal of Public Economics, 88(9), 2099-2123. Senik, C. (2008). Ambition and jealousy: Income interactions in the Old Europe versus the New Europe and the United States. Economica, 75(299), 495-513. University of Essex (2012) Institute for Social and Economic Research and National Centre for Social Research, Understanding Society: Waves 1-2, 2009-2011. 4th Edition. Colchester, Essex: UK Data Archive. SN: 6614. 5

Table 1: Studies testing the relative income effect Study Data Utility Proxy (scale) Reference Group Estimation Method Relative Income Effect Clark and Oswald (1996) McBride (2001) Senik (2004) Ferrer-i-Carbonell (2005) British Household Panel Survey 1991 1994 US General Social Survey (GSS) Russian Longitudinal Monitoring Survey 1994 2000 German Socio- Economic Panel 1992-1997 Luttmer (2005) 1987/1988 & 1992/1994 US National Survey of Families & Households, 1990 Census & Current Population Survey Senik (2008) Clark et al. (2009) Mangyo and Park (2011) European Community Household Panel (ECHP) 14 countries 1994 2001, GSS and European Social Survey. ECHP Denmark Sample & Danish administrative data 1994-2001. China Inequality and Distributive Justice survey, 2004 Overall Job Satisfaction and Pay Satisfaction (1-7) Individual characteristics (predicted income) and spatial (region, occupation, industry) Ordered Probit Negative Happiness (1-3) Individual characteristics (age) Ordered Probit Negative Life Satisfaction (1-5) Life Satisfaction (0-10) Happiness (1-7) Life, Income and Economic Satisfaction (4 to 9 points). Satisfaction with Economic Conditions (1-6) Self-reported health (1-5). Normalised continuous depression scale. Individual characteristics (predicted income) and spatial (region, occupation, industry. Individual characteristics (education, age, sex) and spatial (West and East Germany Spatial (earnings by industry and occupation for Public Use Micro Areas) Individual characteristics (predicted income) and spatial (region, occupation, industry Spatial (neighbourhood and municipality) Subjective groups that you compare yourself to. Objective - spatial (Township, County and Province) Ordered Probit with and without Mundlak RE Ordered Probit with Mundlak OLS with state FE Linear FE. OLS for cross section data. Linear FE OLS with clustered standard errors, with and without geographical FE. Positive Negative Negative Positive in Eastern and Baltic countries. Negative in Western countries. Positive Objective - Insignificant for health. Negative for depression. Subjective - Positive for health. Negative for depression. 6

Table 2: Estimation Results, Comparison of Relative Income Effects Dependent Variable = Life Satisfaction Reference Group: Individual Characteristics Own Income Relative Income Pooled Ordered Probit 0.138*** -0.236*** (0.00579) (0.0248) RE Ordered Probit 0.141*** -0.250*** (0.00805) (0.0387) RE Ordered Probit with Mundlak 0.0407*** -0.288*** (0.0112) (0.0389) FE Ordered Logit 0.0954*** 0.0403 (0.0239) (0.198) Reference Group: Spatial Pooled Ordered Probit 0.135*** 0.00551 (0.00589) (0.0176) RE Ordered Probit 0.136*** 0.0425* (0.00816) (0.0257) RE Ordered Probit with Mundlak 0.0409*** 0.0164 (0.0112) (0.0258) FE Ordered Logit 0.0906*** 0.192** (0.0240) (0.0849) Dependent Variable = GHQ Reference Group: Individual Characteristics Own Income Relative Income Pooled OLS 0.535*** -0.956*** (0.0288) (0.123) RE 0.372*** -0.739*** (0.0291) (0.146) FE 0.133*** -0.481* (0.0382) (0.287) Reference Group: Spatial Pooled OLS 0.503*** 0.284*** (0.0294) (0.0879) RE 0.354*** 0.248*** (0.0295) (0.0943) FE 0.131*** 0.0644 (0.0383) (0.135) Coefficients (standard errors in parentheses). *** p<0.01, ** p<0.05, * p<0.1. Control variables: logarithm of age and age squared, year dummies, highest education level, employment status, logarithm of number of adults and number of children, gender, marital status, country dummies. Mundlak correction includes mean of: number of adults, number of children, monthly income. N = 99,430 7