DO INCOME PROJECTIONS AFFECT RETIREMENT SAVING?

Similar documents
DOG BITES MAN: AMERICANS ARE SHORTSIGHTED ABOUT THEIR FINANCES

WHY DID POVERTY DROP FOR THE ELDERLY?

HOW IMPORTANT IS MEDICARE ELIGIBILITY IN THE TIMING OF RETIREMENT?

ARE PEOPLE CLAIMING SOCIAL SECURITY BENEFITS LATER?

401(k) PLANS AND RACE

SOCIAL SECURITY S FINANCIAL OUTLOOK: THE 2007 REPORT IN PERSPECTIVE

WHY ARE OLDER WORKERS AT GREATER RISK OF DISPLACEMENT?

USING PARTICIPANT DATA TO IMPROVE 401(k) ASSET ALLOCATION

HOW LONG DO UNEMPLOYED OLDER WORKERS SEARCH FOR A JOB?

REDUCING DEFAULT RATES OF REVERSE MORTGAGES

SOCIAL SECURITY S FINANCIAL OUTLOOK: THE 2006 UPDATE IN PERSPECTIVE

THE STRUCTURE OF 401(k) FEES

CAN EDUCATIONAL ATTAINMENT EXPLAIN THE RISE IN LABOR FORCE PARTICIPATION AT OLDER AGES?

NATIONAL RETIREMENT RISK INDEX: HOW MUCH LONGER DO WE NEED TO WORK?

DO INDIVIDUALS KNOW WHEN THEY SHOULD BE SAVING FOR A SPOUSE?

HOW MUCH TO SAVE FOR A SECURE

IS PENSION INEQUALITY GROWING?

HOW DO INHERITANCES AFFECT THE NATIONAL RETIREMENT RISK INDEX?

MODERNIZING SOCIAL SECURITY: HELPING THE OLDEST OLD

HOW RETIREMENT PROVISIONS AFFECT TENURE OF STATE AND LOCAL WORKERS

THE IMPACT OF INFLATION ON SOCIAL SECURITY BENEFITS

HOW DOES 401(K) AUTO-ENROLLMENT RELATE TO THE EMPLOYER MATCH AND TOTAL COMPENSATION?

HOW DOES WOMEN WORKING AFFECT SOCIAL SECURITY REPLACEMENT RATES?

THE IMPACT OF INTEREST RATES ON THE NATIONAL RETIREMENT RISK INDEX

EMPIRICAL REGULARITY SUGGESTS RETIREMENT RISKS

AN ANNUITY THAT PEOPLE MIGHT ACTUALLY BUY

WHAT WILL MY ACCOUNT REALLY BE WORTH? EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE ON HOW RETIREMENT INCOME PROJECTIONS AFFECT SAVING

SHOULD YOU CARRY A MORTGAGE INTO RETIREMENT?

WORKERS RESPONSE TO THE MARKET CRASH: SAVE MORE, WORK MORE?

AN ANNUITY THAT PEOPLE MIGHT ACTUALLY BUY

THE IMPACT OF INTEREST RATES ON THE NATIONAL RETIREMENT RISK INDEX

HOW MUCH DOES HOUSING AFFECT RETIREMENT SECURITY? AN NRRI UPDATE

MEDICARE COSTS AND RETIREMENT SECURITY

HOW HAS THE FINANCIAL CRISIS AFFECTED THE CONSUMPTION OF RETIREES?

The Role of Exponential-Growth Bias and Present Bias in Retirment Saving Decisions

SOCIAL SECURITY S FINANCIAL OUTLOOK: THE 2014 UPDATE IN PERSPECTIVE

THE IMPACT OF RAISING CHILDREN ON RETIREMENT SECURITY

EMPLOYERS (LACK OF) RESPONSE TO THE RETIREMENT INCOME CHALLENGE

THE IMPACT OF AGING BABY BOOMERS ON LABOR FORCE PARTICIPATION

WHY DON T LOWER-INCOME INDIVIDUALS HAVE PENSIONS?

DO YOUNG ADULTS WITH STUDENT DEBT SAVE LESS FOR RETIREMENT?

HOW HAVE WORKERS RESPONDED TO OREGON S AUTO-IRA?

IS WORKING LONGER A GOOD PRESCRIPTION FOR ALL?

ESTIMATING PENSION COVERAGE USING DIFFERENT DATA SETS

IMPACT OF PUBLIC SECTOR ASSUMED RETURNS ON INVESTMENT CHOICES

THE STATE OF PRIVATE PENSIONS: CURRENT 5500 DATA

SOCIAL SECURITY S FINANCIAL OUTLOOK: THE 2011 UPDATE IN PERSPECTIVE

PUBLIC SECTOR WORKERS AND JOB SECURITY

HOUSEHOLDS AT RISK : A CLOSER LOOK AT THE BOTTOM THIRD

JOB TENURE AND THE SPREAD OF 401(K)S

MAKING YOUR NEST EGG LAST A LIFETIME

Article FOUR in a Mercer and Stanford Center on Longevity series Retirement income statements may help avert a looming crisis Authors: Amy Reynolds

A NUDGE ISN T ALWAYS ENOUGH

HOW IMPORTANT ARE INHERITANCES FOR BABY BOOMERS?

THE ANNUITY PUZZLE AND NEGATIVE FRAMING

PENSION WEALTH AND INCOME: 1992,

IS ADVERSE SELECTION IN THE ANNUITY MARKET A BIG PROBLEM?

Using Consequence Messaging to Improve Understanding of Social Security

SOCIAL SECURITY S FINANCIAL OUTLOOK: THE 2018 UPDATE IN PERSPECTIVE

DO STATE ECONOMICS OR INDIVIDUAL CHARACTERISTICS DETERMINE WHETHER OLDER MEN WORK?

DOES SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS LEAD PEOPLE TO RETIRE TOO SOON?

Retirement funding is at a crossroads. For many years, Why Income Should Be the Outcome of a Defined Contribution Plan. Retirement

PENSION COVERAGE AND RETIREMENT SECURITY

NRRI UPDATE SHOWS HALF STILL FALLING SHORT

THE U.K. S AMBITIOUS NEW RETIREMENT SAVINGS INITIATIVE

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES WHAT WILL MY ACCOUNT REALLY BE WORTH? AN EXPERIMENT ON EXPONENTIAL GROWTH BIAS AND RETIREMENT SAVING

HOW MUCH DO OLDER WORKERS VALUE EMPLOYEE HEALTH INSURANCE?

center for retirement research

Mechanisms Behind Retirement Saving Behavior: Evidence From Administrative and Survey Data

The Financial Literacy Initiative. Annamaria Lusardi (Dartmouth College andnber)

STATE AND LOCAL PENSION COSTS: PRE- CRISIS, POST-CRISIS, AND POST-REFORM

SOCIAL SECURITY S FINANCIAL OUTLOOK: THE 2013 UPDATE IN PERSPECTIVE

THE NATIONAL RETIREMENT RISK INDEX: AFTER THE CRASH

401(k) PLANS ARE STILL COMING UP SHORT

HOW SENSITIVE IS PUBLIC PENSION FUNDING TO INVESTMENT RETURNS?

Do Defaults Have Spillover Effects? The Effect of the Default Asset on Retirement Plan Contributions

REDUCING COSTS OF 401(k) PLANS WITH ETFs AND COMMINGLED TRUSTS

SOCIAL SECURITY AND EQUITIES: LESSONS FROM RAILROAD RETIREMENT

THE IMPACT OF LEAKAGES ON 401(K)/IRA ASSETS

A BIRD S EYE VIEW OF THE SOCIAL SECURITY DEBATE

PROBLEMS WITH STATE-LOCAL FINAL PAY PLANS AND OPTIONS FOR REFORM

CAN PBGC SAVE MULTIEMPLOYER PLANS?

DO PUBLIC PENSIONS HELP RECRUIT AND RETAIN HIGH-QUALITY WORKERS?

THE IMPACT OF PUBLIC PENSIONS ON STATE AND LOCAL BUDGETS

PROMOTING WORK: IMPLICATIONS OF RAISING SOCIAL SECURITY S EARLY RETIREMENT AGE

center for retirement research

Gopi Shah Goda. Professional Experience. Education. Fields of Interest. Research

EMPLOYEE MOBILITY AND EMPLOYER-PROVIDED RETIREMENT PLANS. Gopi Shah Goda, Damon Jones, and Colleen Flaherty Manchester

MAKING MAXIMUM USE OF TAX-DEFERRED RETIREMENT ACCOUNTS. Janette Kawachi, Karen E. Smith, and Eric J. Toder

Wealth, money, knowledge: how much do people know? Where are the gaps? What s working? What s next?

THE FINANCIAL CRISIS AND STATE/LOCAL DEFINED BENEFIT PLANS

ARE THE SOCIAL SECURITY TRUST FUNDS MEANINGFUL?

STATE WAGE-PAYMENT LAWS, THE PENSION PROTECTION ACT OF 2006, AND 401(k) SAVING BEHAVIOR. Gary V. Engelhardt CRR WP

MODERNIZING SOCIAL SECURITY: AN OVERVIEW

Dodged a bullet? "Rothification" likely to reduce retirement saving

Ratings and Asset Allocation: An Experimental Analysis 1

What Explains Changes in Retirement Plans during the Great Recession?

A PLANNING GUIDE FOR THE newly retired MANAGING YOUR MONEY. in RETIREMENT

Gopi Shah Goda. Professional Experience. Education. Fields of Interest. Research

WHY DO WOMEN CLAIM SOCIAL SECURITY BENEFITS SO EARLY?

Transcription:

April 2013, Number 13-4 RETIREMENT RESEARCH DO INCOME PROJECTIONS AFFECT RETIREMENT SAVING? By Gopi Shah Goda, Colleen Flaherty Manchester, and Aaron Sojourner* Introduction Americans retirement security increasingly depends on how much they save during their working years. One impediment to making good saving decisions may be a lack of knowledge on how saving translates into income in retirement. To address this issue, the U.S. Congress has considered whether to require 401(k) plans to project the value of a lifetime annuity that the participant could purchase at retirement given his current savings. 1 By explicitly showing the connection between saving and income in retirement, the hope is that workers will generally make better saving decisions. This brief is based on a recent field experiment, conducted with employees of the University of Minnesota, which tested the effect of retirement income projections on saving decisions. 2 The brief proceeds as follows. The first section describes the experimental treatments and the methodology used to analyze the results. The second section presents the results, which address three specific questions: 1) Did subjects receiving the treatments change their saving and by how much? 2) Was any change random or did the treatments improve subjects knowledge and confidence? and 3) Did personal characteristics influence the saving decisions? The final section concludes that providing individuals with retirement income projections, along with related information on retirement planning, could modestly increase saving at low marginal cost. Data and Methodology The field experiment involved nearly 17,000 employees of the University of Minnesota. Compared to the national population, these workers are more highly educated and they have more retirement savings because they are covered by Social Security and one of two generous employer plans. 3 In addition, they can also contribute to a tax-deferred Voluntary Retirement Plan (VRP). The experiment tested the effect of providing employees with age-specific projections of the additional retirement income they could get if they were to make additional contributions to a VRP. 4 * Gopi Shah Goda is a senior research scholar at the Stanford Institute for Economic Policy Research. Colleen Flaherty Manchester and Aaron Sojourner are assistant professors at the Center for Human Resources & Labor Studies, which is part of the University of Minnesota s Carlson School of Management. This brief is adapted from a longer paper (Goda, Manchester, and Sojourner, 2012) that was supported by a grant from the U.S. Social Security Administration, funded as part of the Financial Literacy Center. The authors also acknowledge additional support from the TIAA-CREF Institute and the University of Minnesota s Carlson School of Management.

2 The experiment divided the sample population into four groups a control group and three treatment groups, with each treatment group receiving one of three brochures (see Table 1). 5 The planning treatment brochure provided general information on saving for retirement and a step-by-step guide for signing up or changing contributions to a VRP. The balance treatment brochure added age-specific projections of how hypothetical additional contributions would translate into additional balances at retirement. The income treatment brochure added age-specific projections of how the additional contributions would increase retirement income. 6 Table 1. Content of Brochures by Treatment Group Information Treatment provided Control Planning Balance Income General information X X X Balance projections X X Income projections X To measure the effect on saving, the project used two main outcome measures: 1) whether an employee made any change in his VRP contribution within six months of receiving the brochure; and 2) the change in the amount of the contribution for all employees. Given this approach, the project estimated the effect of the treatments with the following equation: S i = α + T i δ + X i β + η q + ϵ i,d where S i is one of the two outcome measures; T i is a vector of treatment group variables; X i is a vector of controls for age, tenure, salary, gender, faculty status, changes in salary, and campus location; and η q are randomization-quad fixed effects. The error term (ϵ i,d ) is clustered at the department-level (d), the unit of primary randomization. The project also included a supplemental survey conducted after the six-month period to better understand how the treatments affected saving behavior. The supplemental survey included questions on subjects knowledge and confidence about retirement planning and saving. The analysis used the same equation as above to test whether knowledge and confidence were related to the treatments. The survey also collected information on personal characteristics identified in the research literature as either limiting or enhancing retirement saving economic factors such as an individual s ability to cover his expenses, behavioral factors such as procrastination, and cognitive factors such as level of financial literacy. 7 The analysis then interacted these personal characteristics with the treatment indicators to estimate whether the treatments had a different effect on one of the main outcome measures changes in the amount of saving contributions across these characteristics. Experiment Results As indicated above, the experiment was designed to assess three main effects of the treatments: 1) whether, and by how much, subjects changed their saving; 2) whether the treatments increased subjects knowledge and confidence; and 3) whether subjects personal characteristics affected their response to the treatments in terms of saving decisions. 8 How Did the Treatments Affect Saving? The results show that the income treatment had a statistically significant effect on the likelihood that workers would change their contributions and on the amount of their contributions. Compared to the control group, individuals in the income group were 1.2 percentage points more likely to change their contributions during the six-month period following receipt of the brochures 5.3 percent vs. 4.1 percent. The income group as a whole (including both those who changed contributions and those who did not) increased its retirement saving, on average, by $85 more than the control group (see Figure 1 on the next page). 9 However, considering only the individuals who made a change, those in the income group increased their saving by a much larger amount $1,150 more per year than those in the control group. Relative to the control group, individuals in the other two treatment groups the planning group and balance group were also more likely to change their VRP contributions but did not show a statistically significant increase in the amount of saving.

Issue in Brief 3 Figure 1. Effect of Treatments on Change in Average Annual Retirement Saving for All Subjects $90 $60 $30 $0 $36 $60 $85 Planning Balance Income Note: The solid bar indicates the change in annual retirement saving is statistically different from that of the control group at the 5-percent level. Using the planning group instead of the control group as the point of comparison, the balance and income treatments did have a positive effect on saving but the differences were not statistically significant. This finding suggests that it was not the income projections alone, but the combined effect of providing retirement planning information along with the balance and income projections that encouraged the increase in saving for those in the income group. How Did the Treatments Affect the Saving Decision-Making Process? To help determine whether the saving responses to the income treatment were connected to improved knowledge and confidence, the supplemental survey asked several questions connected to the saving decision-making process. The survey response rate was 22 percent, and respondents were somewhat different from the overall group, which warrants caution when interpreting these results. 10 These findings nevertheless offer some useful insights into the saving decisions. The responses show that the income treatment had a beneficial, and statistically significant, effect on knowledge and confidence. Compared to the control group, the income group reported less difficulty finding information about how much to save for retirement and being better informed about retirement planning than they were six months prior. They also reported being more certain about their expected retirement income and more satisfied with their financial condition (see Figure 2). 11 Responses of Figure 2. Effect of Treatments on Subjects Retirement Saving Knowledge and Confidence Difficulty in finding information -12% -6% -5% Better informed than six months prior 7% 8% 20% Retirement income certainty 0% 1% 10% Satisfaction with financial situation 0% -2% 8% -15% -10% -5% 0% 5% 10% 15% 20% Planning Balance Income Note: The solid bars indicate the coefficient is statistically significant at least at the 10-percent level. All effects are measured relative to the control group.

4 Figure 3. Effect of Personal Characteristics on Changes in Annual Retirement Saving for Income Treatment Group, Dollars Difficulty paying bills Prefers to live for today -$198 -$208 } Economic Puts off planning Good at following through -$233 $320 } Behavioral Financially literate Finds planning overwhelming Finds material easy to use $45 $18 $152 } Cognitive -$400 -$200 $0 $200 $400 Note: The size of the bars represent how the treatment effect varies with a one-standard-deviation change in the listed characteristic. The solid bars indicate the coefficient is statistically significant at least at the 5-percent level. individuals in the planning and balance groups were generally not statistically different from those of the control group. What Factors Limited or Enhanced the Income Treatment s Effect? The survey also asked subjects about their personal characteristics economic, behavioral, and cognitive to see if these factors influenced the magnitude of the intervention s effect on saving decisions. The results show that the effect of the income treatment on retirement saving was significantly reduced by a difficulty in paying bills, a strong preference for living pretty much for today, and a tendency to procrastinate, and was significantly enhanced if the respondent was good at following through. Interestingly, cognitive ability and financial literacy generally had little effect, and in no case a statistically significant effect (see Figure 3). 12 Conclusion It should come as no surprise that educational initiatives, such as the treatments used in the field experiment, are not a silver bullet that could solve the nation s retirement saving problem. Only a small percentage of individuals respond to the treatments, so the overall effect on saving is modest, though those who do respond boost their saving by a sizable amount. The income treatment generates the most significant response, likely due to the combined effect of providing balance and income projections in tandem with general information on retirement planning. The income treatment works, in part, by boosting individuals knowledge and confidence. But its effect on saving is limited among those with personal characteristics known to reduce retirement saving. It should be noted that a limited saving response is rational to the extent that increased saving may not be feasible or desirable for everyone depending on individuals personal circumstances. For such individuals, educational initiatives that allow such limited responses could be preferable to other policies designed to promote saving, such as mandates or defaults.

Issue in Brief 5 Endnotes 1 The Lifetime Income Disclosure Act (S. 267 and H.R. 677) and the SAVE Act of 2011 (H.R. 1534) were introduced in 2011 during the 112th Congress. Both of these proposals would require the provision of retirement income projections to participants in retirement saving plans. 2 Goda, Manchester, and Sojourner (2012). 3 Faculty and administrative staff must contribute 2.5 percent of covered earnings, matched by a 13-percent contribution from the University, to a 401(k)-type defined contribution plan. Other employees are covered by a contributory defined benefit pension plan: workers must contribute 5 percent of earnings and at age 65 get a pension equal to 1.7 percent of the average of their highest five years earnings, for each year of service. 4 This study is the first field experiment of its sort in the United States. Song (2012) examined the effects of an educational intervention in China designed to encourage saving by improving financial literacy concerning the benefits of compound interest. 5 Individuals in the balance and income groups were also provided access to an online customization tool designed to mimic the information provided in the printed materials and offer additional features. Such tools are readily available via investment companies websites and would complement any policy initiative on income disclosure by plan sponsors. 6 To reduce the likelihood of contamination across treatment groups, the project chose the department as the unit of randomization. Departments were sorted by size, income and age into four-group clusters. Each department in a quad was randomly assigned to one of the three treatment groups or the no-treatment control group. Every person in a department was either in the control group or the same treatment group. The procedure produced treatment groups balanced on observable characteristics. 8 The study also examined whether the choice of assumptions and other information used in the treatments for projecting saving balances at retirement and income in retirement, such as the rate of return on saving and the dollar amounts of hypothetical contributions, affected the subjects decisions. For details on these findings, see Goda, Manchester, and Sojourner (2012). 9 The control group also increased its saving, so the effects presented here are the amounts over and above their increase. For example, the whole control group increased its saving by $83 annually, so the income group increased its saving by $169 annually. 10 Respondents were disproportionately female, faculty, and VRP participants. Nevertheless, the survey sample is balanced on observable demographics across treatment groups. Observed treatment effects were also substantially higher among survey respondents. 11 Dependent variables are Z-scores of responses, on a scale from 1 to 7, of self-assessments of the particular characteristic. The Z-score for a particular characteristic is the individual s response, less the sample mean, divided by the sample standard deviation. 12 For each measure, the individual s response on a scale from 1 to 7 is turned into a Z-score the response, less the sample mean, divided by the sample standard deviation. The coefficients shown are the change in retirement saving, for those in the income group, resulting from a Z-score one standard deviation higher. 7 For behavioral factors, see Thaler and Benartzi (2004). For cognitive factors, see Lusardi and Mitchell (2007).

6 References Goda, Gopi Shah, Colleen Flaherty Manchester, and Aaron Sojourner. 2012. What Will My Account Really Be Worth? Experimental Evidence on How Retirement Income Projections Affect Saving. WR-873-2. Santa Monica, CA: Rand Financial Literacy Center. Lusardi, Annamaria and Olivia Mitchell. 2007. Financial Literacy and Retirement Preparedness: Evidence and Implications for Financial Education. Business Economics 42(1): 35-44. Lifetime Income Disclosure Act (S. 267; H.R. 677). 2011. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. SAVE Act of 2011 (H.R. 1534). 2011. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Song, Changcheng. 2012. Financial Illiteracy and Pension Contributions: A Field Experiment on Compound Interest in China. Mimeo. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley. Thaler, Richard and Shlomo Benartzi. 2004. Save More Tomorrow: Using Behavioral Economics to Increase Employee Saving. Journal of Political Economy 112(1): S164-S187.

RETIREMENT RESEARCH About the Center The at Boston College was established in 1998 through a grant from the Social Security Administration. The Center s mission is to produce first-class research and educational tools and forge a strong link between the academic community and decision-makers in the public and private sectors around an issue of critical importance to the nation s future. To achieve this mission, the Center sponsors a wide variety of research projects, transmits new findings to a broad audience, trains new scholars, and broadens access to valuable data sources. Since its inception, the Center has established a reputation as an authoritative source of information on all major aspects of the retirement income debate. Affiliated Institutions The Brookings Institution Massachusetts Institute of Technology Syracuse University Urban Institute Contact Information Boston College Hovey House 140 Commonwealth Avenue Chestnut Hill, MA 02467-3808 Phone: (617) 552-1762 Fax: (617) 552-0191 E-mail: crr@bc.edu Website: http://crr.bc.edu 2013, by Trustees of Boston College, Center for Retirement Research. All rights reserved. Short sections of text, not to exceed two paragraphs, may be quoted without explicit permission provided that the authors are identified and full credit, including copyright notice, is given to Trustees of Boston College,. The research reported herein was performed pursuant to a grant from the U.S. Social Security Administration (SSA) funded as part of the Financial Literacy Center. The authors also acknowledge additional support from the TIAA-CREF Institute and the University of Minnesota s Carlson School of Management. The opinions and conclusions expressed are solely those of the authors and do not represent the opinions or policy of SSA, any agency of the federal government, TIAA-CREF, the University of Minnesota, or the Center for Retirement Research at Boston College.